Emily Fyfe Profile Picture

Emily Fyfe

  • efyfe@indiana.edu
  • Psychology Building, PY 351
  • Home Website
  • Assistant Professor
    Psychological and Brain Sciences

Field of study

  • Developmental Science, Cognitive Science, Mathematics Education

Education

  • 2015 – Ph.D. in Psychological Sciences, Vanderbilt University
  • 2010 – B.A. in Psychology, University of Notre Dame

Research interests

  • Children’s knowledge is continuously changing as they observe and interact with people and the world. To that end, my research focuses on the construction and organization of knowledge, with an emphasis on how children think, learn, and solve problems in mathematics. Children exhibit surprisingly intricate knowledge of key math ideas and can generate informal strategies to solve complex problems. Nevertheless, proficiency with mathematics in formal settings is often difficult to achieve. My research is motivated by a question facing cognitive scientists, developmental psychologists, and education practitioners alike: How can we support children’s learning so that it leads to the construction of robust and meaningful knowledge?
  • Thus, my research is in cognitive development with a focus on the development of mathematics knowledge and problem solving. My primary goal is to understand how children think and learn about math, both independently and with instructional guidance. My research not only helps identify basic cognitive processes that support the construction of knowledge, but also examines how to use that information to design effective instructional techniques.

Representative publications

Concreteness fading in mathematics and science instruction: A systematic review (2014)
Emily R Fyfe, Nicole M McNeil, Ji Y Son and Robert L Goldstone
Springer US. 26 (1), 25-Sep

A longstanding debate concerns the use of concrete versus abstract instructional materials, particularly in domains such as mathematics and science. Although decades of research have focused on the advantages and disadvantages of concrete and abstract materials considered independently, we argue for an approach that moves beyond this dichotomy and combines their advantages. Specifically, we recommend beginning with concrete materials and then explicitly and gradually fading to the more abstract. Theoretical benefits of this “concreteness fading” technique for mathematics and science instruction include (1) helping learners interpret ambiguous or opaque abstract symbols in terms of well-understood concrete objects, (2) providing embodied perceptual and physical experiences that can ground abstract thinking, (3) enabling learners to build up a store of memorable images that can be used …

“Concreteness fading” promotes transfer of mathematical knowledge (2012)
Nicole M McNeil and Emily R Fyfe
Learning and Instruction, 22 (6), 440-448

Recent studies have suggested that educators should avoid concrete instantiations when the goal is to promote transfer. However, concrete instantiations may benefit transfer in the long run, particularly if they are “faded” into more abstract instantiations. Undergraduates were randomly assigned to learn a mathematical concept in one of three conditions: generic, in which the concept was instantiated using abstract symbols, concrete in which it was instantiated using meaningful images, or fading, in which it was instantiated using meaningful images that were “faded” into abstract symbols. After learning, undergraduates completed a transfer test immediately, one week later, and three weeks later. Undergraduates in the fading condition exhibited the best transfer performance. Additionally, undergraduates in the generic condition exhibited somewhat better transfer than those in the concrete condition, but this …

Benefits of practicing 4= 2+ 2: Nontraditional problem formats facilitate children’s understanding of mathematical equivalence (2011)
Nicole M McNeil, Emily R Fyfe, Lori A Petersen, April E Dunwiddie and Heather Brletic‐Shipley
Child development, 82 (5), 1620-1633

This study examined whether practice with arithmetic problems presented in a nontraditional problem format improves understanding of mathematical equivalence. Children (M age = 8;0; N = 90) were randomly assigned to practice addition in one of three conditions: (a) traditional, in which problems were presented in the traditional “operations on left side” format (e.g., 9 + 8 = 17); (b) nontraditional, in which problems were presented in a nontraditional format (e.g., 17 = 9 + 8); or (c) no extra practice. Children developed a better understanding of mathematical equivalence after receiving nontraditional practice than after receiving traditional practice or no extra practice. Results suggest that minor differences in early input can yield substantial differences in children’s understanding of fundamental concepts.

Feedback both helps and hinders learning: The causal role of prior knowledge (2016)
Emily R Fyfe and Bethany Rittle-Johnson
Journal of Educational Psychology, 108 (1), 82

Feedback can be a powerful learning tool, but its effects vary widely. Research has suggested that learners’ prior knowledge may moderate the effects of feedback; however, no causal link has been established. In Experiment 1, we randomly assigned elementary schoolchildren (N= 108) to a condition based on a crossing of 2 factors: induced strategy knowledge (yes vs. no) and immediate, verification feedback (present vs. absent). Feedback had positive effects for children who were not taught a correct strategy, but negative effects for children with induced knowledge of a correct strategy. In Experiment 2, we induced strategy knowledge in all children (N= 101) and randomly assigned them to 1 of 3 conditions: no feedback, immediate correct-answer feedback, or summative correct-answer feedback. Again, feedback had negative effects relative to no feedback. Results provide evidence for a causal role of prior …

Emerging understanding of patterning in 4-year-olds (2013)
Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Emily R Fyfe, Laura E McLean and Katherine L McEldoon
Journal of Cognition and Development, 14 (3), 376-396

Young children have an impressive amount of mathematics knowledge, but past psychological research has focused primarily on their number knowledge. Preschoolers also spontaneously engage in a form of early algebraic thinking—patterning. In the current study, we assessed 4-year-old children's knowledge of repeating patterns on two occasions (N = 66). Children could duplicate and extend patterns, and some showed a deeper understanding of patterns by abstracting patterns (i.e., creating the same kind of pattern using new materials). A small proportion of the children had explicit knowledge of pattern units. Error analyses indicated that some pattern knowledge was apparent before children were successful on items. Overall, findings indicate that young children are developing an understanding of repeating patterns before school entry.

Early math trajectories: Low‐income children's mathematics knowledge from ages 4 to 11 (2017)
Bethany Rittle‐Johnson, Emily R Fyfe, Kerry G Hofer and Dale C Farran
Child Development, 88 (5), 1727-1742

Early mathematics knowledge is a strong predictor of later academic achievement, but children from low‐income families enter school with weak mathematics knowledge. An early math trajectories model is proposed and evaluated within a longitudinal study of 517 low‐income American children from ages 4 to 11. This model includes a broad range of math topics, as well as potential pathways from preschool to middle grades mathematics achievement. In preschool, nonsymbolic quantity, counting, and patterning knowledge predicted fifth‐grade mathematics achievement. By the end of first grade, symbolic mapping, calculation, and patterning knowledge were the important predictors. Furthermore, the first‐grade predictors mediated the relation between preschool math knowledge and fifth‐grade mathematics achievement. Findings support the early math trajectories model among low‐income children.

Benefits of “concreteness fading” for children's mathematics understanding (2015)
Emily R Fyfe, Nicole M McNeil and Stephanie Borjas
Learning and Instruction, 35 104-120

Children often struggle to gain understanding from instruction on a procedure, particularly when it is taught in the context of abstract mathematical symbols. We tested whether a “concreteness fading” technique, which begins with concrete materials and fades to abstract symbols, can help children extend their knowledge beyond a simple instructed procedure. In Experiment 1, children with low prior knowledge received instruction in one of four conditions: (a) concrete, (b) abstract, (c) concreteness fading, or (d) concreteness introduction. Experiment 2 was designed to rule out an alternative hypothesis that concreteness fading works merely by “warming up” children for abstract instruction. Experiment 3 tested whether the benefits of concreteness fading extend to children with high prior knowledge. In all three experiments, children in the concreteness fading condition exhibited better transfer than children in the other …

An alternative time for telling: When conceptual instruction prior to problem solving improves mathematical knowledge (2014)
Emily R Fyfe, Marci S DeCaro and Bethany Rittle‐Johnson
British journal of educational psychology, 84 (3), 502-519

<h3 class="gsh_h3">Background</h3> The sequencing of learning materials greatly influences the knowledge that learners construct. Recently, learning theorists have focused on the sequencing of instruction in relation to solving related problems. The general consensus suggests explicit instruction should be provided; however, when to provide instruction remains unclear. <h3 class="gsh_h3">Aims</h3> We tested the impact of conceptual instruction preceding or following mathematics problem solving to determine when conceptual instruction should or should not be delayed. We also examined the learning processes supported to inform theories of learning more broadly. <h3 class="gsh_h3">Sample</h3> We worked with 122 second‐ and third‐grade children. <h3 class="gsh_h3">Method</h3> In a randomized experiment, children received instruction on the concept of math equivalence either before or after being asked to solve and explain challenging equivalence problems with feedback. <h3 class="gsh_h3">Results</h3> Providing …

Arithmetic practice can be modified to promote understanding of mathematical equivalence (2015)
Nicole M McNeil, Emily R Fyfe and April E Dunwiddie
Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 (2), 423

This experiment tested if a modified version of arithmetic practice facilitates understanding of math equivalence. Children within 2nd-grade classrooms (N= 166) were randomly assigned to practice single-digit addition facts using 1 of 2 workbooks. In the control workbook, problems were presented in the traditional “operations= answer” format (eg, 4+ 3= __) and were organized pseudorandomly throughout the workbook. In the modified workbook, problems were presented with operations on the right side (eg, __= 4+ 3), the equal sign was sometimes replaced by relational words (eg,“is equal to”), and problems were organized by equivalent sums such that several problems in a row would all have the same sum. Children who used the modified workbook constructed a better understanding of math equivalence than did children who used the control workbook. This advantage persisted approximately 5–6 months …

Beyond numeracy in preschool: Adding patterns to the equation (2015)
Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Emily R Fyfe, Abbey M Loehr and Michael R Miller
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 31 101-112

Patterns are a pervasive and important, but understudied, component of early mathematics knowledge. In a series of three studies, we explored (a) growth in children's pattern knowledge over the pre-K year (N = 65), (b) the frequency of pattern activities reported by parents (n = 20) and teachers (n = 5) relative to other mathematical activities, and (c) changes in 4-year-old children's pattern knowledge after brief experience generating or receiving explanations on patterns (N = 124). Together, these studies illustrate the types of experiences preschool children are receiving with patterns and how their pattern knowledge changes over time and in response to explanation. Young children are able to succeed on a more sophisticated pattern activity than they are frequently encouraged to do at home or at school.

It pays to be organized: Organizing arithmetic practice around equivalent values facilitates understanding of math equivalence (2012)
Nicole M McNeil, Dana L Chesney, Percival G Matthews, Emily R Fyfe, Lori A Petersen, April E Dunwiddie ...
Journal of Educational Psychology, 104 (4), 1109

This experiment tested the hypothesis that organizing arithmetic fact practice by equivalent values facilitates children's understanding of math equivalence. Children (M age= 8 years 6 months, N= 104) were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 practice conditions:(a) equivalent values, in which problems were grouped by equivalent sums (eg, 3+ 4= 7, 2+ 5= 7, etc.),(b) iterative, in which problems were grouped iteratively by shared addend (eg, 3+ 1= 4, 3+ 2= 5, etc.), or (c) no extra practice, in which children did not receive any practice over and above what they ordinarily receive at school and home. Children then completed measures to assess their understanding of math equivalence. Children who practiced facts organized by equivalent values demonstrated a better understanding of math equivalence than children in the other 2 conditions. Results suggest that organizing arithmetic facts into conceptually related groupings …

The influence of relational knowledge and executive function on preschoolers’ repeating pattern knowledge (2016)
Michael R Miller, Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Abbey M Loehr and Emily R Fyfe
Journal of Cognition and Development, 17 (1), 85-104

Children’s knowledge of repeating patterns (e.g., ABBABB) is a central component of early mathematics, but the developmental mechanisms underlying this knowledge are currently unknown. We sought clarity on the importance of relational knowledge and executive function (EF) to preschoolers’ understanding of repeating patterns. One hundred twenty-four children aged 4 to 5 years old were administered a relational knowledge task, 3 EF tasks (working memory, inhibition, set shifting), and a repeating pattern assessment before and after a brief pattern intervention. Relational knowledge, working memory, and set shifting predicted preschoolers’ initial pattern knowledge. Working memory also predicted improvements in pattern knowledge after instruction. The findings indicated that greater EF ability was beneficial to preschoolers’ repeating pattern knowledge and that working-memory capacity played a …

When feedback is cognitively-demanding: The importance of working memory capacity (2015)
Emily R Fyfe, Marci S DeCaro and Bethany Rittle-Johnson
Instructional Science, 43 (1), 73-91

Feedback is generally considered a beneficial learning tool, and providing feedback is a recommended instructional practice. However, there are a variety of feedback types with little guidance on how to choose the most effective one. We examined individual differences in working memory capacity as a potential moderator of feedback type. Second- and third-grade children (N = 64) solved unfamiliar math problems prior to receiving instruction. Children received verification feedback on their answers (outcome-feedback) or on their strategies (strategy-feedback). Working memory capacity moderated the effect of feedback type on procedural transfer—the ability to solve novel problems. Children with lower working memory capacity benefitted less from strategy-feedback than outcome-feedback, whereas children with higher working memory capacity benefitted similarly from the two types of feedback. Results …

Easy as ABCABC: Abstract language facilitates performance on a concrete patterning task (2015)
Emily R Fyfe, Nicole M McNeil and Bethany Rittle‐Johnson
Child development, 86 (3), 927-935

The labels used to describe patterns and relations can influence children's relational reasoning. In this study, 62 preschoolers (M<sub>age</sub> = 4.4 years) solved and described eight pattern abstraction problems (i.e., recreated the relation in a model pattern using novel materials). Some children were exposed to concrete labels (e.g., blue‐red‐blue‐red) and others were exposed to abstract labels (e.g., A‐B‐A‐B). Children exposed to abstract labels solved more problems correctly than children exposed to concrete labels. Children's correct adoption of the abstract language into their own descriptions was particularly beneficial. Thus, using concrete learning materials in combination with abstract representations can enhance their utility for children's performance. Furthermore, abstract language may play a key role in the development of relational thinking.

Providing feedback on computer-based algebra homework in middle-school classrooms (2016)
Emily R Fyfe
Computers in Human Behavior, 63 568-574

Homework is transforming at a rapid rate with continuous advances in educational technology. Computer-based homework, in particular, is gaining popularity across a range of schools, with little empirical evidence on how to optimize student learning. The current aim was to test the effects of different types of feedback on computer-based homework. In the study, middle school students completed a computer-based pretest, homework assignment, and posttest containing challenging algebraic problems. On the homework assignment, students were assigned to different feedback conditions. In Experiment 1 (N = 103), students received no feedback or correct-answer feedback after each problem. In Experiment 2 (N = 143), students received (1) no feedback, (2) correct-answer feedback, (3) try-again feedback, or (4) explanation feedback after each problem. For students with low prior knowledge, feedback resulted in …

Edit your profile